Please download to get full document.

View again

of 9

Comparing the Effects of L1 and L2 Definition on Incidental Vocabulary Learning through Listening to Stories

Comparing the Effects of L1 and L2 Definition on Incidental Vocabulary Learning through Listening to Stories
0 views9 pages
All materials on our website are shared by users. If you have any questions about copyright issues, please report us to resolve them. We are always happy to assist you.
Documenttranscript
  Comparing the Effects of L1 and L2 Definition on Incidental Vocabulary Learning through Listening to Stories Seyed Mohammad Reza Amirian Hakim Sabzevari University, Sabzevar, Iran Gholamreza Zareian Hakim Sabzevari University, Sabzevar, Iran Sima Nour Hakim Sabzevari University, Sabzevar, Iran  Abstract   —  Vocabulary is an essential component of language proficiency which provides the basis for learners’ performance in other skills. This study investigated the effect of three kinds of definition conditions, that is L1, L2, and L1+L2 on incidental vocabulary knowledge of EFL learners. To this aim, three experimental and one control groups took part in the current study so as to examine the effectiveness of definitions with 96 target words through listening. The participants were 74 fourth-grade high school students. Eight stories with each with 12 target words (total 96 words) were given to the learners. Participants in each group listened to eight short stories under one of the three mentioned conditions for experimental groups, with no definition offered for control group. They all answered vocabulary list, eight immediate post-tests and eight delayed posttests of vocabulary. The data were analyzed using t- tests and one-way  ANOVA  for both immediate and delayed post-tests. The results indicated that definition groups significantly outperformed the other group in terms of vocabulary acquisition on both immediate and delayed retention of target words. However, the findings showed a significant loss from the immediate to the delayed post-tests. These findings are discussed and implications are offered for foreign language syllabus designers and instructors. Index Terms  —  incidental vocabulary learning, L1 and L2 definition, listening comprehension, story- telling, glossing I.   I  NTRODUCTION  Vocabulary learning has been extensively researched in recent years since the fact that vocabulary plays a pivotal role in the communication and academic lives of foreign language learners is irrefutable (Al-Dersi, 2013, Belisle, 1997) and there is no doubt that learning vocabulary is a key factor for language mastery (Schmitt, 2008). Even some researchers such as Gass (1999) equate learning a second language with its vocabulary learning. Moreover, vocabulary learning is supposed to be a multidimensional phenomenon which includes the integration of different kinds of knowledge along with gaining various levels of ability to make use of that knowledge in communication (Paribakht and Wesche, 1999). Certainly, improving a rich vocabulary is a first priority for both L1 and L2 learners without which their other language skills suffer significantly. That is why Wilkins (1972) many years ago argued that “While  without grammar very little can be conveyed, without vocabulary nothing can be conveyed ” (  p. 111). Because of the critical role of vocabulary in learning a foreign language, learners should draw on a variety of strategies to learn vocabulary. Hunt and Beglar (1998) believe that there are three approaches to improving vocabulary learning: incidental learning, explicit instruction, and independent strategy development. Among these three approaches, incidental vocabulary learning which includes extensive reading and listening is considered as a significant part of learning vocabulary. Most of the papers in the domain of incidental vocabulary learning (Wesche & Paribakht, 1999) have considered incidental learning as something that is learned without specific focus of attention in a classroom context. According to Paribakht and Wesche (1999), incidental vocabulary learning refers to the fact that learners concentrate on meaning of listening and reading contexts instead of focusing on intentional vocabulary learning without the learn er’s attempt to learn the words through methods such as guessing from context, learning through definitions, etc. Even though there have been a growing number of studies concerning learning vocabulary through reading (Pulido, 2007), learning vocabulary through listening, is also a promising source of vocabulary acquisition. For example, some researchers have shown that students learn vocabulary through listening to stories or lectures (Brett, Rothlein & Hurley, 1996; Elley, 1989). ISSN 1798-4769Journal of Language Teaching and Research, Vol. 7, No. 3, pp. 604-612, May 2016DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.17507/jltr.0703.24 © 2016 ACADEMY PUBLICATION  Generally speaking, L2 listening has received relatively little research attention (Vandergrift, 2007), and this is true about incidental vocabulary acquisition as well. Thus, fewer studies have been carried out in the context of listening as compared with reading. Some early vocabulary studies have included listening, but they explored how auditory stimuli can reinforce acquisition from reading (e.g. Kelly, 1992), rather than acquisition from listening only. Recently, a few vocabulary studies have measured learning from listening directly. Their findings suggest that listening leads to even smaller gains than reading does (Brown et al., 2008; Vidal, 2011; as cited in (Van Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013). Another common way of initially providing the meaning of lexical items is to explain them in target language. It has  been argued that short, direct, unambiguous, and simple definitions (glosses) work best (Ellis, 1995; Nation, 2001). In any case, learners’ interest in the aural input and the comprehensibility of that input are vital for vocabulary acquisition to occur according to these researchers. This study aimed at investigating the effectiveness of different definition types, that is, L1 definition, L2 definition, and L1+ L2 definition on incidental vocabulary learning of Iranian high school students through listening to stories.   II.   L ITERATURE R  EVIEW    A. Incidental and Intentional Learning in L2 Vocabulary Literature Vocabulary is learned either incidentally or intentionally. While incidental learning can be applied to both abstract and factual declarative knowledge, intentional learning is only applicable to factual knowledge (Hulstijn, 2003). Ellis (1999) describes the distinction between incidental and intentional learning as follows: The distinction between incidental and intentional learning is based on the distinction between focal and peripheral   attention. Intentional learning requires focal attention to be placed deliberately on the linguistic code (i.e., on form or form-meaning connections), while incidental learning requires focal attention to be placed on meaning (i.e., message content) but allows peripheral attention to be directed at form (p.45). Many vocabularies are learned incidentally through extensive reading and listening. therefore, encouraging learners to read and listen extensively can provide them with great opportunities and ample input the is necessary for vocabulary learning. According to Huckin and Coady (1999) except for the first few thousand most common words, L2 vocabulary learning predominantly happens through extensive reading with guessing of the unknown words. This process is incidental learning of vocabulary for the acquisition of new words and is the by-product of reading. Gass (1999) points out that words are more likely to be learned incidentally if (a) there are recognized cognates between the native and the target languages, (b) there is important L2 exposure, or (c) other L2 related words are known. Brown,Waring, and Donkaewbua (2008) compared vocabulary learning from reading and listening and found that the difference between reading and listening was that the former lead to significantly more vocabulary learning. Even though both Vidal (2011) and Brown et al. (2008) held a comparative analysis of learning through spoken and written input, the primary concern is the actual uptake from listening. This is important in the fact that the two tests provided by Brown et al. led to very different scores; this demonstrates that incidental vocabulary learning is more complex than could be revealed by these tests only. The lack of vocabulary knowledge assessment in listening studies is surprising. Therefore, this study on the comparison of different gloss types aims at measuring the effect of providing definitions in listening to stories which is not investigated so far.  B. Meanings and Importance of Glosses The concept of glossing has been largely studied by researchers in recent years. Traditionally, a gloss is a definition or meaning for L2 learners to improve reading comprehension. Nation (1983) defined glosses as short definitions. Segler (2001) pointed to them as translations or brief explanations of difficult or technical texts (e.g. unusual words) and categorized glosses into textual glosses, pictorial (visual) and aural glosses and various combinations (as cited in Jalali & Neiriz, 2012). Roby (1991) expressed that “glosses supply what is perceived to be deficient in a reader’s procedural or declarative knowledge "(p.183). Lomicka (1998) defines glosses as short definitions or explanations that are often supplied to facilitate reading and comprehension processes for L2 learners. Researchers (e.g., Yoshii, 2006; Nation, 2002) pointed to glossing as one of the most effective tools for increasing noticing that increase vocabulary learning among ESL/EFL learners. Glossing is the easiest way for perceiving the meanings of words as they appear in context, since it does not even demand the effort of searching and then choosing the suitable meaning out of several possible ones, which is required  by dictionary look-up. Researchers generally agree that glosses facilitate reading comprehension and short-term vocabulary retention (Kost et al., 1999).   C. Reasons for Using Glosses Glosses can have at least four advantages for learning. First, readers can use glosses to understand new words more accurately by preventing incorrect guessing. Guessing meaning from context can be risky and difficult because of readers' lack of language or reading strategies (e.g., Bensoussan and Laufer, 1984; Hulstijn, 1992; Kruse, 1979; Nation, 2001; Stein, 1993). Second, glossing can reduce interruption while reading is in process. Since glossing supplies JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE TEACHING AND RESEARCH605© 2016 ACADEMY PUBLICATION  definitions for low frequency words, L2 readers do not have to constantly look them up (Nation, 1990; Nation, 2001). Third, glosses assist readers in making a bridge between previous knowledge or experience and new information in the text. In other words, interactions among gloss, reader, and text develop comprehension and retention of the content of the text. Besides these points, glosses in key words can aid readers recall their background knowledge and connect it to the text (Stewrat and Cross, 1993). Fourth, glosses can make students less dependent on their teachers, allowing for greater autonomy. Since not all students have problems with the same words, they can look up just the words they do not know (Jacobs, 1994; Nation, 1990). Some studies have indicated that students prefer to have glosses in their L2 language reading materials (e.g., Jacobs, Dufon and Fong, 1994). In each case, the use of vocabulary glosses in L2 reading materials is a common practice (Holley and King, 1971; Jacobs, et al., 1994; as cited in Hee Ko, 2005).  D. L1 versus. L2 Gloss There is a controversy over the effectiveness of L1 and L2 glosses and studies have produced conflicting results with some reporting no difference while others indicating the advantage of one gloss type over the other type (Chen, 2002; Jacob et al., 1994; Miyasako, 2002). In an early study Jacobs et al. (1994) did a study on 85 English-speaking  participants who were studying Spanish as a second language compared to compare L1 with L2 glosses. The results of the immediate test demonstrated that either L1 or L2 were better than no gloss; however, no significant difference was found between L1 and L2 glosses. In a similar study, Chen (2002) examined L1 and L2 glosses with 85 college freshmen in Taiwan who were studying English as a second language in three groups - (1) L1 (Chinese) gloss; (2) L2 (English) gloss; and (3) No gloss. The findings proved that the L2 group outperformed the no gloss group, and that the difference between L1 and L2 glosses was not significant (as cited in Fahimipour & Hashemian, 2013). Miyasako (2002)'s study found the advantage of one gloss type over the other which is contrary to previous studies. Comparing the effectiveness of L1 and L2 glosses, Miyasako concluded that the L2 gloss groups outperformed the L1 gloss groups. In addition, L2 glosses appeared to be more effective for higher-proficiency level learners, whereas L1 glosses were more effective for lower proficiency learners (as cited in Yoshii, 2006). As the results of the comparisons between L1 and L2 are inconclusive, there is a need for more studies comparing the effectiveness of L1 and L2 especially in listening skill which is an under-researched area which is going to be addressed in the present study.  E. Listening Comprehension and Short Stories Although listening was almost ignored in language learning and teaching for a long time, recently it has achieved its active and communicative value. Due to the developments in technology, these days the teachers and the researchers make a profit from several listening instruments in the classroom to hone learners' listening comprehension ability.  Nonetheless, students in each level may have problems listening to audio programs. In order to enhance this skill, teachers have looked for different techniques and strategies to teach and receive requested results. According to Ghanimi, Arjmandi, and Rahimy (2014), a very demanding listening task is audio story task, designed as supplementary material to language text books that can eliminate the students' listening problem. However, listening has remained a tough skill to teach students because setting listening task is considered time-consuming and boring. It seems that the first step in overcoming the barriers would be understanding the language spoken in a context. If the learners do not listen attentively, there will be some difficulties in understanding. Larsen-Freeman (2000, p. 148) states that, "being able to figure out the speaker's or writer's intentions is part of being communicatively competent." Generally speaking, the focal issue of the current research project is to determine which definition condition is more effective in aiding vocabulary retention. This research will not only analyze different forms of the definitions, but also give a more detailed description of the effect of definitions on incidental vocabulary acquisition. Additionally, this study investigates to introduce audio story that can be enjoyable material for the learners into EFL classes and also can maximize the students' exposure to suitable listening program. Therefore, to clarify what is in an EFL high school context, the present research aimed to answer the following questions: 1. Do learners in experimental groups (vocabulary definition) perform better than learners in control group (no definition) on vocabulary posttests? 2. Does providing Ll, L2, and L1 + L2 definition affect immediate vocabulary retention of learners? 3. Does providing Ll, L2, and L1+L2 definition affect long term retention of learners? III.   M ETHOD    A. Participants   This study was conducted on seventy four (N=74) female students studying at Ansarifard high school in Sabzevar, Iran. All the students of the survey were of roughly the same age (17-18) and were studying English as a compulsory subject during guidance and high school education. All of them were native speakers of Persian and English was a foreign language for them. The class met twice a week for 90 minutes and was a requirement for graduation. They had  passed grade eleven and they had the same English learning background. Hence, it was assumed that the participants formed a suitable sample for the intended experiment. This was also determined by their classwork and homework. They were randomly assigned to three experimental and one control group. Each group got on a definition condition 606JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE TEACHING AND RESEARCH© 2016 ACADEMY PUBLICATION   based on four definition types: L1 definition, L2 definition, L1 + L2 definition, and no definition. The treatment took 12 sessions.  B. Research Design The design of this study was an experimental one with vocabulary definition being the independent variable and vocabulary learning the dependent variable. During the experiment, the students listened to eight short stories during one month in the first semester of 2014 under one of the four conditions: L1 definition (Persian language), L2 definition (English language), L1 + L2definition (Persian and English language), and no definition. The participants in three definition groups were considered as the experimental groups and the participants in no gloss group were presumed to  be control group in this study. After listening to the stories, the participants were asked to answer the immediate post- tests. Then the delayed post tests were conducted after two weeks one by one. In this study different types of definitions (L1, L2, L1 + L2, and no definition) were identified as the independent variable. The dependent variable included  participants’ sc ores on immediate and delayed vocabulary posttests and the design of the study was experimental with  both post- tests. The tests measured learners’ receptive and productive vocabulary knowledge.   C. Materials and Instruments To conduct the present study, four instruments were employed to collect the data. The instruments were the same for all the participants. They consisted of a vocabulary list used before treatment, immediate post- tests (Production & Recognition), delayed post-tests (Production & Recognition), and eight short stories from graded readers at appropriate difficulty levels that are explained below.  D. Vocabulary Lists To ensure the learners are not aware of the target words prior to the treatment, a vocabulary list was administered to the learners in order to measure their knowledge of the target words. The test contained 100 words selected from eight short stories. The participants were instructed to put a check mark by any words they knew and provide a short written explanation in L1.Conseq uently, 96 words out of 100 words were selected. To assess students’ learning of the target words throughout the study, recognition and production tests were employed .  E. Immediate Post Tests Participants received two vocabulary posttests: one immediately after the treatment and the other two weeks later. This study used a two-week span following similar studies (Chun & Plass, 1996; Kost et al., 1999; Yoshii & Flaitz, 2002). The purpose of using immediate post- test was to measure the participants’ vocabula ry knowledge. The immediate post- test required the students to write down the meanings of the given English words in Persian, and make sentences with the given target words. Eight immediate post tests were administered to the participants at various stages of the experiment. Each post- test was composed of two parts: a recognition and a production test in the form of definition supply with making sentences. The format of the definition supply test was in the way that participants were asked to mark the words they remembered and provide their meanings in L1 (Persian). The recognition test consisted of the target words with four multiple choice answers for each item. The participants should select the most suitable definition of the given word through four choices written in L2 (English). Each test included a total of 12 target words, and the students received one point for each correct answer, making the maximum score of 12 points.  F. Delayed Post Tests The same eight immediate post- tests were administered to the students as delayed post- tests two weeks after the first immediate post- test one by one. They were applied after listening and answering the immediate tests at the beginning of the session. In other words, after two weeks students should answer to two tests: a new test immediately after listening to the story and the delayed post- test of the previous tests. The purpose of using delayed post- test was to measure the participants’ vocabulary retention and long term memory. Immediate post -test, on the other hand, aimed to measure the learners’ target word knowledge incidentally acquired during the listening activities.   G. Short Stories Eight short stories with 96 head words (n=96) were selected and prepared for the participants in the incidental vocabulary learning condition in graded reader form. Stories were chosen according to the proficiency level of all the  participants. They were similar in length and difficulty. Although the stories’ name was familiar for the participants, measures were taken to make sure that learners had not already read the stories in new version and with new vocabulary in order to minimize the effect of old vocabulary. In each story, definition of about 20-25 (new and distracter) words were given. Short stories were adapted into four different forms: a short story with no definition; a short story with L1 definition (definitions or synonyms in Persian); a short story with L2 definition (definitions or synonyms in English); and a short story with L1 + L2 definition (definitions or synonyms in English and Persian). A multiple-choice listening test consisting of eight items (four items with choices in Persian and four items with English choices) were given to the students after listening to the stories.  H. Data Collection Procedure JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE TEACHING AND RESEARCH607© 2016 ACADEMY PUBLICATION  Four classes of the same grade were chosen to participate in the study. One of the classes was randomly adopted as the control group and the other classes as the experimental groups. One hundred target vocabulary items from eight short stories from graded readers were considered and given to the students as vocabulary list. Among these, 96 words were covered in the stories. In attempting to ensure the target words were unfamiliar, only items that had not appeared in any of the previous course books were chosen. The students were told about vocabulary list and also about the immediate post-tests in advance but, the delayed post tests were not mentioned and participants took them unexpectedly. This was done to create conditions for incidental vocabulary learning. The students were required to listen to eight short stories during one month. The stories were told live by the teacher to the students. Before treatment, a vocabulary list was given to the students and then they were told about listening to eight short stories and answering the vocabulary tests. They took the first announced immediate post -test one week later the vocabulary list and delayed post tests were administered two weeks after the first immediate post-test.  I. Data Analysis Procedure To test the hypotheses of the study, some descriptive and inferential data analysis procedures were performed. The data in this study was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 21).On the basis of the aforementioned research questions, the data was analyzed using one-way ANOVA for test of vocabulary in immediate and delayed post-test for research questions 3 and 4. In order to understand if three aforementioned definition conditions were more effective than no definition condition, that is to answer research questions 1, two separate independent samples t-tests were used. For all the analysis, the alpha level was .05.In all treatment sessions the effect of two strategies of learning vocabulary was measured: 1) immediately after treatment and 2) two weeks later so as to determine the long term retention effect. In this study, descriptive statistics were used to determine the mean and standard deviation of each group on posttests (immediate and delayed). IV.   R  ESULTS    A. The First Research Question The first question of the current study concerned investigating whether providing L1, L2, and L1+ L2 definition (experimental groups) in comparison with no definition (control group) affect learners’ incidental vocabulary learning differently As displayed in Table 1, the mean of the experimental group (  M= 63.31, SD= 13.81) and   that of the control group (  M  = 5.50, SD =4.560) showed that the means were quite different. In fact, the experimental group outperformed the control group on the immediate posttest of vocabulary retention. T ABLE 1. D ESCRIPTIVE S TATISTICS FOR THE E XPERIMENTAL AND C ONTROL G ROUP ON I MMEDIATE P OSTTEST  Control experimental N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Mean mmediate 1 54 63.31 13.081 1.780 2 20 5.50 4.560 1.020  Note: 1= experimental, 2= control However, to see if this difference is statistically significant an independent sample t-test   is used and the results are displayed in Table 4.2. T ABLE 2. I  NDEPENDENT -S AMPLES T-T EST FOR THE E XPERIMENTAL AND C ONTROL GROUPS ON I MMEDIATE P OST TEST  Levene's Test for Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means F Sig. t df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Difference Std. Error Difference 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference Lower Upper immediate Equal variances assumed 7.835 .007 19.264 72 .000 57.815 3.001 51.832 63.798 Equal variances not assumed 28.182 71.894 .000 57.815 2.051 53.725 61.904 The descriptive statistics of mean and standard deviation of each group was calculated and equal variances was not assumed was reported. Results of the independent samples t-test ( t   (72) = 28.182,  P  <.05) indicated that there was a significant difference between the experimental and control gr  oups’ mean scores on immediate  post-test of vocabulary retention. T ABLE 3 D ESCRIPTIVE S TATISTICS FOR E XPERIMENTAL AND C ONTROL G ROUPS ON THE D ELAYED P OSTTEST  Control experimental N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Mean delayed 1 54 44.93 17.615 2.397 2 20 4.20 3.302 .738  Note: 1= experimental, 2= control 608JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE TEACHING AND RESEARCH© 2016 ACADEMY PUBLICATION
Advertisement
MostRelated
View more
We Need Your Support
Thank you for visiting our website and your interest in our free products and services. We are nonprofit website to share and download documents. To the running of this website, we need your help to support us.

Thanks to everyone for your continued support.

No, Thanks
SAVE OUR EARTH

We need your sign to support Project to invent "SMART AND CONTROLLABLE REFLECTIVE BALLOONS" to cover the Sun and Save Our Earth.

More details...

Sign Now!

We are very appreciated for your Prompt Action!

x